The Open Ornithology Journal




(Discontinued)

ISSN: 1874-4532 ― Volume 13, 2020
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Painted Redstarts (Myioborus Pictus) Attack Larger Prey when Using Flush-Pursue Strategy



Piotr G. Jablonski1, 2, Sang-im Lee1, 3, *
1 Laboratory of Behavioral Ecology and Evolution, School of Biological Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, South Korea
2 Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Wilcza 64, 00-679 Warsaw, Poland
3 DGIST, School of Undergraduate Studies, Daegu 42988, South Korea

Abstract

Introduction:

Prey escape reaction in insects is an antipredatory adaptation that is mediated by prey neural escape circuits with specific sensory properties.

Methods:

Certain insectivorous birds, flush-pursuers, exploit this visual sensitivity by employing conspicuous pivoting movements of spread tail and wings to flush the prey into the air where it is available for chase in aerial pursuits. Although it is known that this strategy increases the number of insects attacked, no information has been published on the size distribution of arthropods attacked using flush-pursue strategy vs. traditional gleaning and pecking off substrate strategy.

Results:

Based on one season of observational data of foraging redstarts (Myioborus pictus) we show that prey items that were flushed and chased were on average larger than prey pecked off of substrates.

Conclusion:

This may be one of the benefits from flush-pursue foraging – a strategy that is probably costly in terms of energy demands.

Keywords: Myioborus pictus, Predator-prey interactions, Flush-pursuers, Sensory exploitation, Foraging, “Rare enemy”, Prey size.


Article Information


Identifiers and Pagination:

Year: 2018
Volume: 11
First Page: 34
Last Page: 38
Publisher Id: TOOENIJ-11-34
DOI: 10.2174/1874453201811010034

Article History:

Received Date: 10/11/2017
Revision Received Date: 20/02/2018
Acceptance Date: 14/3/2018
Electronic publication date: 30/03/2018
Collection year: 2018

© 2018 Jablonski and Lee.

open-access license: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Public License (CC-BY 4.0), a copy of which is available at: (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode). This license permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.


* Address correspondence to this author at the School of Undergraduate Studies, DGIST, Daegu 42988, South Korea, Tel: 82 53 785 6613; E-mail: sangim@dgist.ac.kr





1. INTRODUCTION

Some species of insectivorous birds, the flush-pursuers (sensu) [1Remsen JV, Robinson SK. A classification scheme for foraging behavior of birds in terrestrial habitats. Stud Avian Biol 1990; 13: 144-60.], use conspicuous pivoting movements of spread tail and wings to exploit prey antipredatory escape reactions [2Edmunds M. Defence in Animals: A survey of anti-predator defences 1974., 3Evans DL, Schmidt JO. Insect defenses: Adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators 1990.]. These birds make a cryptic prey item conspicuous by flushing it into the air where it can be captured following an aerial pursuit, and by using this foraging method they increase the number of prey items delivered to their nestlings [4Barber MB, Barber DR, Jablonski PG. Painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). In: Poole A, Gill F, Eds. The Birds of North America, No 528 2000.-14Sherry TW, Homes RT. American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla). In: Poole A, Gill F, Eds. Birds of North America, No 227, The Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, PA, and the American Ornithologists Union, Washington DC: 1997].

The painted redstart (Myioborus pictus) is one of such flush-pursuing species and it uses the conspicuous wing and tail displays during foraging [8Jablonski PG. A rare predator exploits prey escape behavior: The role of tail-fanning and plumage contrast in foraging of the painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). Behav Ecol 1999; 10: 7-14.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/10.1.7]
, 15Jabłonski PG. Sensory exploitation of prey: Manipulation of the initial direction of prey escapes by a conspicuous “rare enemy”. Proc Biol Sci 2001; 268(1471): 1017-22.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1623] [PMID: 11375085]
-17Jabłoński PG, Strausfeld NJ. Exploitation of an ancient escape circuit by an avian predator: Relationships between taxon-specific prey escape circuits and the sensitivity to visual cues from the predator. Brain Behav Evol 2001; 58(4): 218-40.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000057565] [PMID: 11964498]
]. When Myioborus redstarts use their displays, prey-pursuit flights are more frequent then they are during non-display-foraging [8Jablonski PG. A rare predator exploits prey escape behavior: The role of tail-fanning and plumage contrast in foraging of the painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). Behav Ecol 1999; 10: 7-14.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/10.1.7]
] and they are able to feed their nestlings more frequently [8Jablonski PG. A rare predator exploits prey escape behavior: The role of tail-fanning and plumage contrast in foraging of the painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). Behav Ecol 1999; 10: 7-14.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/10.1.7]
, 10Mumme RL. Scare tactics in a neotropical warbler: white tail feathers enhance flush pursuit foraging performance in the slate-throated redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Auk 2002; 119: 1024-35., 18Jabłoński PG, Lasater K, Mumme RL, et al. Habitat-specific sensory-exploitative signals in birds: Propensity of dipteran prey to cause evolution of plumage variation in flush-pursuit insectivores. Evolution 2006; 60(12): 2633-42.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1554/06-363.1] [PMID: 17263122]
, 19Mumme RL, Galatowitsch ML, Jabłoński PG, Stawarczyk TM, Cygan JP. Evolutionary significance of geographic variation in a plumage-based foraging adaptation: An experimental test in the slate-throated redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Evolution 2006; 60(5): 1086-97.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2006.tb01185.x] [PMID: 16817547]
]. Although it is known that this strategy increases the number of insects attacked, no information has been published on the size distribution of arthropods attacked using flush-pursue strategy vs. traditional gleaning and pecking off substrate strategy. We ask here whether prey items that were flushed and chased differ in size from those that were pecked off of substrates. The answer will expand our knowledge about predator-prey interactions in the systems with flush-pursue predators.

2. METHODS

The research was conducted in the Cave Creek Canyon, Chiricahua Mountains, near the Southwestern Research Station, Portal, AZ (latitude: 31.866; longitude: 109.238; UTM zone 12). During the breeding season of 1997 (May - June) foraging redstarts were followed to describe food-size distributions for birds during aerial chasing or pecking off the prey. During field work concerning breeding phenology, singing and territorial behavior we opportunistically collected foraging observations of birds across the whole study area. Although many birds were not individually banded, foraging observations were sparsely distributed in space and time and we assumed that they were independent from each other and probably from 24 different birds. The observations were conducted along the Cave Creek with its tributaries and along the East Turkey Creek, altogether comprising the total length of about 17 km. Redstart breeding territories and nests were located along these creeks (Fig. 1) (nest locations from 1997). When a foraging bird was spotted the observer attempted to watch it closely through binoculars while audio-recording the observations for later translation. Only occasionally, it was possible to determine the size of the prey captured by a foraging bird. In total, we collected such information for 32 prey items (average 1.3 per bird) captured during aerial chases after flushed prey and for 52 prey items (average 2.2 per bird) captured by picking off the substrate (trunks, branches and leaves). Because the determination of size is not always possible the foraging sequences in which it was achieved several times for one bird were interspersed in time and across different trees. Therefore we regarded them as independent events. We coded prey size captured by birds in five size ranks: 1. prey smaller than 2mm; 2. prey larger than 2 mm and smaller than or equal to half of redstart bill length; 3. prey larger than half but smaller than or equal to one bill length; 4. prey larger than one bill length and smaller than or equal to 1.5 bill lengths; 5. prey larger than 1.5 bill lengths. Using a Mann-Whitney U test, we tested the null hypothesis that median prey size rank does not differ between the two hunting techniques: pecking off the substrate and chasing prey in air. Using G-test, we also compared distributions of the prey sizes between these two hunting techniques. We used sequential-Bonferroni correction of significance levels for these two tests (k=2; Rice, 1989). The taxonomic category of the prey was difficult to ascertain in most cases.

3. RESULTS

The size rank of prey chased in air (mean, [median, (minimum-maximum)]; 2.6 [3Evans DL, Schmidt JO. Insect defenses: Adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators 1990.] (1-5), n=32) was larger (Mann Whitney test; Z1=2.68, P=0.008) than the size rank of prey pecked off the substrate (1.8) [1Remsen JV, Robinson SK. A classification scheme for foraging behavior of birds in terrestrial habitats. Stud Avian Biol 1990; 13: 144-60.] (1-5),(n=52), and the two prey size distributions differed between each other (Fig. 1); (G4 = 25.71), (P=0.00008). Most prey pecked off substrates consisted of small unrecognizable insects. Among 10 insects pecked that were recognizable there were 6 caterpillars, 3 spiders, and 1 cicada nymph. Among 8 insects chased that were recognizable there were 3 flies, 2 cicadas, 1 moth and 2 Hemipterans. Caterpillars were the largest prey items pecked off the substrate. The details of the diet of redstart’s nestlings during breeding season are going to be reported separately, but these taxonomic groups of prey are commonly brought to nestlings.

4. DISCUSSION

In addition to previous analyses on foraging Painted redstarts and Slate-throated redstarts [8Jablonski PG. A rare predator exploits prey escape behavior: The role of tail-fanning and plumage contrast in foraging of the painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). Behav Ecol 1999; 10: 7-14.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/10.1.7]
, 10Mumme RL. Scare tactics in a neotropical warbler: white tail feathers enhance flush pursuit foraging performance in the slate-throated redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Auk 2002; 119: 1024-35., 19Mumme RL, Galatowitsch ML, Jabłoński PG, Stawarczyk TM, Cygan JP. Evolutionary significance of geographic variation in a plumage-based foraging adaptation: An experimental test in the slate-throated redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Evolution 2006; 60(5): 1086-97.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2006.tb01185.x] [PMID: 16817547]
], that indicated benefit from flush-pursuing owing to the larger number of attacks on prey, here we focused on the size of prey estimated in the field conditions. The recorded size distribution of prey suggested that Painted redstarts may additionally benefit because the flush-pursue strategy results in on average larger prey than the prey pecked off the substrates. Jablonski and Strausfeld [16Jablonski PG, Strausfeld NJ. Exploitation by a recent avian predator of an ancient arthropod escape circuit: Prey sensitivity and elements of the displays by predators. Brain Behav Evol 2000; 56: 94-106.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000006680] [PMID: 11111136]
, 17Jabłoński PG, Strausfeld NJ. Exploitation of an ancient escape circuit by an avian predator: Relationships between taxon-specific prey escape circuits and the sensitivity to visual cues from the predator. Brain Behav Evol 2001; 58(4): 218-40.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000057565] [PMID: 11964498]
] and Jablonski et al. [18Jabłoński PG, Lasater K, Mumme RL, et al. Habitat-specific sensory-exploitative signals in birds: Propensity of dipteran prey to cause evolution of plumage variation in flush-pursuit insectivores. Evolution 2006; 60(12): 2633-42.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1554/06-363.1] [PMID: 17263122]
] have shown that models of redstarts approaching a variety of potential prey at the present study site may evoke escape responses in a variety of medium- and large-sized prey such as flies, leafhoppers or moths. In another flush-pursuing species, the American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla), Robinson & Holmes [11Robinson SK, Holmes RT. Foraging behavior of forest birds: The relationships among search tactics, diet, and habitat structure. Ecology 1982; 63: 1918-31.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1940130]
] and Keast et al. [9Keast A, Pearce L, Saunders S. How convergent is American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla, Parulinae) with flycatchers (Tyrannidae) in morphology and feeding behavior? Auk 1995; 112: 310-25.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/4088719]
] also suggested that the majority of prey pursued in flight was medium or large size, while the prey pecked off the substrate by warblers often contained small and energetically unrewarding insects (in addition to some large caterpillars during specific time of year). Similarly, flush-pursue strategy of tail-flicking in the Hooded Warbler (Setophaga citrina) results in different types of prey brought to the nestlings [20Mumme RL. White tail spots and tail flicking behavior enhance foraging performance in the Hooded Warbler. Auk 2014; 131: 141-9.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1642/AUK-13-199.1]
]. Hence, it seems that regardless of whether flush-pursuers use tail-flicking or spread-tail pivoting the birds capture more and different, possibly more energy-rewarding, insects. It is possible that temperature may affect this difference in prey size between flush-pursuing and gleaning because it is possible that insects may flush more easily in higher temperatures. Future studies should address this issue.

Fig. (1)
Comparison of size distributions of prey caught by pecking off substrates and by chasing flying prey. Ranks: 1 – prey smaller than 2 mm; 2 – prey larger than 2 mm and smaller than or equal to half of redstart bill length; 3 - prey larger than half and smaller than or equal to one bill length; 4 – prey larger than one bill length and smaller than or equal to 1.5 bill lengths; 5 – prey larger than 1.5 bill lengths. The inset shows a foraging Painted redstart in a display pose (photo by P. G. Jablonski).


CONCLUSION

In summary, this is the first report based on direct observations of foraging birds that documents how flush-pursue foraging method may not only increase the number of insects available for pursuit, but that those insects are of medium and large size, providing energetic benefits that may possibly help to offset the costs of aerial chases by flush-pursuers.

ETHICS APPROVAL AND CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE

Not applicable.

HUMAN AND ANIMAL RIGHTS

No human or animal were used in this study that on bases of this research.

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Not applicable.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The field work was funded by the Maria Sklodowska-Curie Joint Fund II (PAN/EPA-96-241), the Kosciuszko Foundation, the American Museum of Natural History (Chapman Grants), the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour, and the Committee for Scientific Research (KBN grant 6 PO4F 030 08). Analysis and manuscript preparation was supported by the KRF project nr. 0409-20080118, the NRF project 2016R1D1A1B03934340, the Brain Korea 21 Project, and the grant (3344-20080067) from the College of Natural Sciences, Seoul National University and by the DGIST Start-up Fund Program of the Ministry of Science, ICT and Future Planning (2018010100). The staff of the Southwestern Research Station - W.Sherbrooke, E. Sherbrooke, A. Oritz, L.Oritz, and S.Cox, as well as station volunteers are gratefully acknowledged.

REFERENCES

[1] Remsen JV, Robinson SK. A classification scheme for foraging behavior of birds in terrestrial habitats. Stud Avian Biol 1990; 13: 144-60.
[2] Edmunds M. Defence in Animals: A survey of anti-predator defences 1974.
[3] Evans DL, Schmidt JO. Insect defenses: Adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators 1990.
[4] Barber MB, Barber DR, Jablonski PG. Painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). In: Poole A, Gill F, Eds. The Birds of North America, No 528 2000.
[5] Fitzpatrick JW. Foraging behavior of neotropical tyrant flycatchers. Condor 1980; 82: 43-57.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1366784]
[6] Holmes RT, Recher HF. Search tactics of insectivorous birds foraging in an Australian eucalypt forest. Auk 1986; 103: 515-30.
[7] Jablonski P. Adaptive significance of colour patterns in Painted Redstart. J Ornithol 1994; 135(Suppl.): 147.
[8] Jablonski PG. A rare predator exploits prey escape behavior: The role of tail-fanning and plumage contrast in foraging of the painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). Behav Ecol 1999; 10: 7-14.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/10.1.7]
[9] Keast A, Pearce L, Saunders S. How convergent is American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla, Parulinae) with flycatchers (Tyrannidae) in morphology and feeding behavior? Auk 1995; 112: 310-25.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/4088719]
[10] Mumme RL. Scare tactics in a neotropical warbler: white tail feathers enhance flush pursuit foraging performance in the slate-throated redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Auk 2002; 119: 1024-35.
[11] Robinson SK, Holmes RT. Foraging behavior of forest birds: The relationships among search tactics, diet, and habitat structure. Ecology 1982; 63: 1918-31.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1940130]
[12] Sherry TW. Ecological and evolutionary inferences from morphology, foraging behavior, and diet of sympatric insectivorous neotropical flycatchers (Tyrannidae). PhD Thesis 1982.
[13] Sherry TW. Comparative and dietary ecology of sympatric insectivorous neotropical flycatchers (Tyrannidae). Ecol Monogr 1984; 54: 313-38.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1942500]
[14] Sherry TW, Homes RT. American Redstart (Setophaga ruticilla). In: Poole A, Gill F, Eds. Birds of North America, No 227, The Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, PA, and the American Ornithologists Union, Washington DC: 1997
[15] Jabłonski PG. Sensory exploitation of prey: Manipulation of the initial direction of prey escapes by a conspicuous “rare enemy”. Proc Biol Sci 2001; 268(1471): 1017-22.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1623] [PMID: 11375085]
[16] Jablonski PG, Strausfeld NJ. Exploitation by a recent avian predator of an ancient arthropod escape circuit: Prey sensitivity and elements of the displays by predators. Brain Behav Evol 2000; 56: 94-106.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000006680] [PMID: 11111136]
[17] Jabłoński PG, Strausfeld NJ. Exploitation of an ancient escape circuit by an avian predator: Relationships between taxon-specific prey escape circuits and the sensitivity to visual cues from the predator. Brain Behav Evol 2001; 58(4): 218-40.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000057565] [PMID: 11964498]
[18] Jabłoński PG, Lasater K, Mumme RL, et al. Habitat-specific sensory-exploitative signals in birds: Propensity of dipteran prey to cause evolution of plumage variation in flush-pursuit insectivores. Evolution 2006; 60(12): 2633-42.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1554/06-363.1] [PMID: 17263122]
[19] Mumme RL, Galatowitsch ML, Jabłoński PG, Stawarczyk TM, Cygan JP. Evolutionary significance of geographic variation in a plumage-based foraging adaptation: An experimental test in the slate-throated redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Evolution 2006; 60(5): 1086-97.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2006.tb01185.x] [PMID: 16817547]
[20] Mumme RL. White tail spots and tail flicking behavior enhance foraging performance in the Hooded Warbler. Auk 2014; 131: 141-9.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1642/AUK-13-199.1]
Track Your Manuscript:


Endorsements



"Open access will revolutionize 21st century knowledge work and accelerate the diffusion of ideas and evidence that support just in time learning and the evolution of thinking in a number of disciplines."


Daniel Pesut
(Indiana University School of Nursing, USA)

"It is important that students and researchers from all over the world can have easy access to relevant, high-standard and timely scientific information. This is exactly what Open Access Journals provide and this is the reason why I support this endeavor."


Jacques Descotes
(Centre Antipoison-Centre de Pharmacovigilance, France)

"Publishing research articles is the key for future scientific progress. Open Access publishing is therefore of utmost importance for wider dissemination of information, and will help serving the best interest of the scientific community."


Patrice Talaga
(UCB S.A., Belgium)

"Open access journals are a novel concept in the medical literature. They offer accessible information to a wide variety of individuals, including physicians, medical students, clinical investigators, and the general public. They are an outstanding source of medical and scientific information."


Jeffrey M. Weinberg
(St. Luke's-Roosevelt Hospital Center, USA)

"Open access journals are extremely useful for graduate students, investigators and all other interested persons to read important scientific articles and subscribe scientific journals. Indeed, the research articles span a wide range of area and of high quality. This is specially a must for researchers belonging to institutions with limited library facility and funding to subscribe scientific journals."


Debomoy K. Lahiri
(Indiana University School of Medicine, USA)

"Open access journals represent a major break-through in publishing. They provide easy access to the latest research on a wide variety of issues. Relevant and timely articles are made available in a fraction of the time taken by more conventional publishers. Articles are of uniformly high quality and written by the world's leading authorities."


Robert Looney
(Naval Postgraduate School, USA)

"Open access journals have transformed the way scientific data is published and disseminated: particularly, whilst ensuring a high quality standard and transparency in the editorial process, they have increased the access to the scientific literature by those researchers that have limited library support or that are working on small budgets."


Richard Reithinger
(Westat, USA)

"Not only do open access journals greatly improve the access to high quality information for scientists in the developing world, it also provides extra exposure for our papers."


J. Ferwerda
(University of Oxford, UK)

"Open Access 'Chemistry' Journals allow the dissemination of knowledge at your finger tips without paying for the scientific content."


Sean L. Kitson
(Almac Sciences, Northern Ireland)

"In principle, all scientific journals should have open access, as should be science itself. Open access journals are very helpful for students, researchers and the general public including people from institutions which do not have library or cannot afford to subscribe scientific journals. The articles are high standard and cover a wide area."


Hubert Wolterbeek
(Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands)

"The widest possible diffusion of information is critical for the advancement of science. In this perspective, open access journals are instrumental in fostering researches and achievements."


Alessandro Laviano
(Sapienza - University of Rome, Italy)

"Open access journals are very useful for all scientists as they can have quick information in the different fields of science."


Philippe Hernigou
(Paris University, France)

"There are many scientists who can not afford the rather expensive subscriptions to scientific journals. Open access journals offer a good alternative for free access to good quality scientific information."


Fidel Toldrá
(Instituto de Agroquimica y Tecnologia de Alimentos, Spain)

"Open access journals have become a fundamental tool for students, researchers, patients and the general public. Many people from institutions which do not have library or cannot afford to subscribe scientific journals benefit of them on a daily basis. The articles are among the best and cover most scientific areas."


M. Bendandi
(University Clinic of Navarre, Spain)

"These journals provide researchers with a platform for rapid, open access scientific communication. The articles are of high quality and broad scope."


Peter Chiba
(University of Vienna, Austria)

"Open access journals are probably one of the most important contributions to promote and diffuse science worldwide."


Jaime Sampaio
(University of Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, Portugal)

"Open access journals make up a new and rather revolutionary way to scientific publication. This option opens several quite interesting possibilities to disseminate openly and freely new knowledge and even to facilitate interpersonal communication among scientists."


Eduardo A. Castro
(INIFTA, Argentina)

"Open access journals are freely available online throughout the world, for you to read, download, copy, distribute, and use. The articles published in the open access journals are high quality and cover a wide range of fields."


Kenji Hashimoto
(Chiba University, Japan)

"Open Access journals offer an innovative and efficient way of publication for academics and professionals in a wide range of disciplines. The papers published are of high quality after rigorous peer review and they are Indexed in: major international databases. I read Open Access journals to keep abreast of the recent development in my field of study."


Daniel Shek
(Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong)

"It is a modern trend for publishers to establish open access journals. Researchers, faculty members, and students will be greatly benefited by the new journals of Bentham Science Publishers Ltd. in this category."


Jih Ru Hwu
(National Central University, Taiwan)


Browse Contents




Webmaster Contact: info@benthamopen.net
Copyright © 2023 Bentham Open